ESSENTIAL HYPERTENSION: A PSYCHO-SOMATIC FEATURE OR A
PSYCHO-SOMATIC DISEASE?
Vadim S. Rotenberg vadir@post.tau.ac.il, V. V. Alexeyev.
|
Group |
General peripheral resistance (dyn-sec-cm5) |
Minute volume (litres) |
Heart output (mm3) |
Delta sleep % |
REM sleep % |
VI scale MMPI |
2nd |
-173.6 |
+ 1.0 |
+ 14.0 |
+0.1 |
+ 5.0 |
57 |
1st |
+265.8 |
-0.34 |
-2.6 |
+ 12.0 |
-6.7 |
49 |
p< 0.05 |
p< 0.05 |
p<0.05 |
p<0.01 |
p<0.01 |
p< 0.025 |
As follows from the Table, this approach has disclosed two types of reaction to emotional-intellectual stress in sufferers from essential hypertension. In both instances stress led to a rise in the arterial pressure compared to the background value but in the first group this was due to a rise in the general peripheral vascular resistance with a relative drop in the cardiac output and the minute blood volume. In the second group this resulted from an expansion in the cardiac output accompanied by some drop in the peripheral vascular resistance. It is highly significant that the rise in arterial pressure was accompanied by a rise in the minute cardiac volume only in the second group patients. In the first group patients the minute volume did not register a significant post-stress change. On the post-stress night their delta-sleep time grew longer and their REM sleep time shorter. The second group patients exhibited a longer REM sleep time, a shorter delta-sleep time and a negative REM-delta sleep correlation (r=0.47). Both groups showed a negative correlation between the delta-sleep time and scale D and a positive correlation between the REM sleep time and scale Pa MMPI (r=+0.83) (see Fig 1) .
A control group of eight subjects, in whom the third night was registered without previous stress, did not exhibit the above sleep structure changes. These two types of sleep change strikingly resemble two types of reaction of healthy students to examination stress disclosed in studies of Rotenberg and Arshavsky (1979). Directly before the examination all students exhibited a rise in the arterial pressure, pulse frequency and facial muscle tension - evidence of emotional tension. Following success in the examinations in some of the students all these indices got back to the control level in the first 30 minutes (Group A) whereas the others (Group B) even exhibited a trend toward their increase although the stressful situation was gone. (It would be pertinent to recall here Kozlovskaya's data on persistent hypertension following an aversive situation in animals which reacted to this situation on lines of renunciation of search). In Group A students, who showed a typical rapid normalization of all indices, the subsequent sleep had an increased proportion of delta-sleep (compared to the control night unaffected by examination stress) whereas in Group B students it showed a longer REM sleep time, REM sleep having a negative correlation with delta-sleep. It was assumed that Group B was characterized by unproductive anxiety-type pre-examination tension similar to the state of renunciation of search, which conditioned the stabilization of physiological indices even after the successful ending of the aversive situation, leading to a rise in the REM sleep requirement. Indirect evidence of the above was the fact that on the post-examination evening the second group students poorly coped with intellectual problems - worse than in the control study, worse than on the morning after a sleep with a prolonged REM period and worse than the first group students. Meanwhile, the increase in the delta-sleep proportion in Group A students could be due to their high intellectual and perceptive load of the previous day. Some data indicate that such a load increases the delta-sleep requirement (Home, Wormsley, 1976). This also stands in good agreement with the experimental evidence of the role of delta-sleep in the organization and retention of learned material (Latash, Manov, 1975).
Figure 1.
Correlations between Physiological and Psychological Indices in Individuals with a Rise in Arterial Pressure
Comparing the results of the study of emotional stress in healthy students and individuals with initial forms of essential hypertension the authors feel bound to recall that in the latter category stress also bore a mixed (emotional and intellectual) character. It was directly addressed to the intellect and mnestic functions (the students were offered to resolve problems or recall data which for a long time had not been restored in their immediate memory). The authors therefore have every reason to believe that the increase in the delta-sleep period in the first group of individuals with high arterial pressure reflects an intensification of the intellectual search activity corresponding to a normal reaction to the intellectual load of the healthy Group A students. It follows that the rise in arterial pressure also represents not more than a component of psychophysiological mobilization necessary in solving intellectual problems - a physiological component of search activity. This assumption is also confirmed by the specific features of post-stress haemo-dynamics: a moderate rise in peripheral vascular resistance constitutes an adequate reaction of healthy individuals to stress if the cardiac output grows smaller and the minute volume remains at a permanent level in the process. Such a reaction of the cardiovascular system reveals the normal functioning of the reflex mechanism of autoregulation, when a rise in the blood pressure through the carotid sinus system activates the bulbar mechanisms of regulation of the cardiovascular system (which in the experiment, incidentally, led to a rise in the blood pressure during the REM sleep (Averill et al., 1970)). Meanwhile, the rise in arterial pressure in the second group of examined patients formed a physiological concomitant to the maladaptive reaction that proceeded on lines of renunciation of search. Consequently, only these subjects can be regarded as psycho-somatic patients. This assumption is supported by a number of facts:
1. The REM sleep phase, which compensates for the state of renunciation of search, grew longer only in this group.
2. The above hypothesis is also supported by the post-stress haemodynamic changes proper in this group of patients: the arterial pressure rises predominantly due to cardiac output whereas the total peripheral resistance tends towards a drop. Incidentally, precisely such a change in haemodynamic indices was detected in dogs exposed to a chaotic administration of electric shocks (Anderson, Jingling, 1978) - a widespread method of inducing experimental neurosis, learned helplessness and in general all states which the authors group as renunciation of search. The dog's behaviour exactly corresponded to this state being marked by falls on their front paws and freezing - in the periods between the irregularly supplied conditional signal and electric shock as well as outside this set of stimuli. Anderson and Jingling believe it important that the acute aversive situation leading to a rise in arterial pressure and pulse frequency does not necessarily result in a rise in general peripheral resistance.
In the examined second group patients the increase in the heart output was attended, despite a drop in the peripheral resistance, by an increase in the minute volume - a prognostically unfavorable symptom revealing that the mechanisms of auto-regulation of the cardiovascular system are upset. A group of researchers led by Brod (1960) assumes that the increase in the minute volume of, and the bloodstream passing through, the muscles is accompanied by a decrease in the bloodstream passing through the kidneys. In this case there is no increase in the general resistance to the bloodstream (Feifar, Widimsky, 1960) - a phenomenon observed at later stages of essential hypertension. The reason for such a type of reaction of the haemodynamic system is acute emotional stress (Brod, 1963). The majority of modern researchers, basing themselves on the theory suggested by Guyton et al. (1974), assume that such a hyperkinetic variant of arterial hypertension eventually passes into hypertension with high peripheral vascular resistance because of the protective myogenetic stenosis of the arterioles in response to the increase in the blood inflow to the organs and in the oxygen pressure in the tissues, which exceeds metabolic requirements. This secondary rise in peripheral vascular resistance furnishes evidence of a severe decompensation, is related to the activation of the humoral factors that maintain high arterial pressure and is fraught with severe somatic complications. This makes it important to observe that humoral regulation in ontogeny is formed before neuro-reflex (partly repeating phylogeny) whereas the state of renunciation of search is regarded by the authors as regressive - a reversion to an ontogenetically earlier type of behavioural reaction to stress. If one also is to consider that all second group patients had histories of early-age psychogenies it could be assumed that these diseases had promoted the retention (on lines of imprinting) of, first, a behavioural reaction to stress on the principle of renunciation of search and, second, of the attending rise in arterial pressure predominantly due to factors of humoral regulation.
It can thus be assumed that the rise in arterial pressure stemming mostly from a rise in peripheral resistance with a stable minute volume is typical of search behaviour and of mobilization manifesting itself in activity. Meanwhile, a rise in blood pressure because of an increase in cardiac output and minute volume, even in the case of a drop in peripheral resistance at the early stages is typical of unproductive emotional tension, which accompanies the state of renunciation of search.
These assumptions were found to be almost coincidental with the clinico-psychological correlates disclosed by Danies and Moses (cited from Schors, 1970).
1. An excessive rise in arterial pressure conditioned by an increase in peripheral vascular resistance and the frequency of cardiac contractions is accompanied by a predominant reaction of the fury type.
2. Manifest anxiety represents the basic concomitant to a moderate rise in arterial pressure, which is related to an increase in peripheral resistance with normal stroke volume and frequency of cardiac contractions.
3. Suppressed anxiety is characterized by a lesser rise in arterial pressure, which stems from an increase in stroke volume and frequency of cardiac contractions with normal peripheral resistance.
Apparently, the first two behavioural types, which are characterized by a rise in peripheral resistance, can be regarded as ones which include components of search activity.
The above leads the authors to assume that hypertension proper as a psychosomatic disease is only the rise in arterial pressure which accompanies renunciation of search and is characterized, at least at the early stages of the disease, by a relative predominance of minute volume (hyperkinetic type) over peripheral resistance.
The rise in blood pressure that goes with search behaviour should be considered to be a variant of normal psychophysiological component of search behaviour rather than a pathology.
However, the identification of two polar types, far from excluding, emphasizes the existence of a third (intermediate or mixed) type. Its presence is confirmed by correlations between the analyzed indices.
Continued analysis should disclose whether some instances of stable high blood pressure, resistant to therapy and not leading to complications for many years, should be classed with the normal state. As is known, rises in arterial pressure are not infrequently discovered in members of particularly responsible trades or professions (traffic controllers, flyers, etc.) whose functioning requires for success an ability for stable concentration. In many instances the treatment of such subjects happens to be ineffective and if, in spite of all this, the doctors succeed in bringing down their blood pressure this fairly often exercises a negative effect on the physical state and vocational adequacy of these people. A question arises: Doesn't the pressure in such cases drop due to a drop in necessary (and, on the whole, useful for the individual) search activity? If this is the case such subjects can be treated very carefully or not treated at all. However, the problem owes its complexity to the fact that the individuals in whom high blood pressure is a normal physiological correlate of search activity can, in certain circumstances, also contract hypertension as an actual psycho-somatic disease. This is why differentiation of these two forms of rise in arterial blood pressure - normal and pathological - is of topical interest. At the present stage such differentiation will be facilitated by a study of sleep structure. Future researchers, however, will attempt to find other stable criteria for identifying these forms.
Literature:
Afanasjeva, N. B.; Beshimov, A. B. (1974): The Influence of Neurotropic Drugs on Somato-Autonomic Manifestations of the Emotional Stress Evoked by Psychological Stress-Stimuli. In: Neuropharmacological Regulation of the System Processes. Ed. A. V. Valdman, Leningrad, 169-176.
Alexander, F. (1950): Psychosomatic Medicine, New York.
Alexeyev, V. V. (1980): Some Cerebral Mechanisms of the Pathogenesis of Early Stages of Hypertension. Candidate of Medical Sciences Degree Thesis, Moscow (in Russian).
Ammon, G. (1979): Entwurf eines Dynamisch-Psychiatrischen Ich-Struktur-Konzepts. In: G. Ammon (Hrsg.) Handbuch der Dynamischen Psychiatrie, Bd. 1, (Miinchen: Ernst Reinhardt).
Anderson, D. E.; Jingling, J. E, (1978): Total Peripheral Resistance Changes in Dogs during Aversive Classical Conditioning. In: The Pavlovian Journal of Biolog. Science, Vol. 13, No 4, 241-246.
Averill, D. B.; Feerario, C. At.; McCubbin, I. W. etal. (1975): Paradoxical Hypertension during Sleep in Dogs with Buffer Nerve Section (Abstract). In: "Physiologist", 18, 125.
Bastiaans, Y. (1969): The Role of Aggression in the Genesis of Psychosomatic Disease. In: J. Psychosom. Res., Vol. 13, 307-314.
Brad, Y. (1960): Hemodynamic Response to Stress and its
Bearing on the Hemodynamic Basis of Essential Hypertension. In:
Proc. of the Prague Symposium, The Pathogenesis of Essential
Hypertension
- (1960): Hemodynamic Basis of Acute Pressor Reaction and
Hypertension, Brit. Heart. J. Vol. XXV, 227-245.
Dickinson, C. J. (1965): Neurogenic Hypertension, (Oxford: Blackwell)
Engel, G., Schmale, A. (1967): Psychoanalytic Theory of Somatic Disorders. In: J. Amer. Psy-choan. Assoc., Vol. 15, 344-365.
Feifar, Z.; Widimsky,]. (1960): Juvenile Hypertension. In: Proc. of joint WHO-Czechoslovak Cardiological Society Symposium on Pathogenesis of Essential Hypertension. (Prague: State Medical Pub). House) 33-42.
Folkow, B.; HeilE. (1971): Circulation (N.Y.: Oxford University Press)
Guyton, A.; Coleman, .; Cowley,Jr.; Manning, Jr.; Normal, Jr.; Ferguson,J. (1974): A Systems Analysis Approach to Understanding Long-Range Arterial Blood Pressure Control and Hypertension. Circulation Res. 35, 159-176.
Home, J. A.; Walmsley, B. (1976): Daytime Visual Eoad and the Effects upon Human Sleep. In: Psychophysiology, Vol. 13, 115-120.
Koch, C. (1980): Empirisch-Statistische Uberpriifung einiger Klinischer Hypothesen zur Diffe-renzierung von psychosomatisch erkrankten und neurotisch-depressiven Patienten einerseits und psychosomatisch erkrankten und neurotisch depressiven Patienten im Vergleich zu einer ausgesuchten Kontrollstichprobe andererseits. In: Dyn. Psychiat. (13) 332-346.
Kozlovskaya, M. M.: Personal Communication.
Latash, L. P.; Manov, G. A. (1975): Relation of Delta-Sleep and Phasic Component of REM Sleep with Retention and Recall of Verbal Material Learned just before Sleep. In: Human Physiology, Vol. 1, No 2.
McClelland, D. (1979): Inhibited Power Motivation and High Blood Pressure in Men. In: Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol. 88, No 2, 182-190.
Miller, N. E. (1976): Learning, Stress and Psychosomatic Symptoms. In: Acta Neurobiol. Exptl., Vol. 36, 141-156.
Pickering, G. W. (1955): High Blood Pressure (London)
Rotenberg, V. S.; Arshavsky, V. V. (1979): REM Sleep, Stress and Search Activity. In: Waking and Sleeping, Vol. 3, No 3, 235-244.
(1979): Search Activity and its Impact on Experimental and Clinical Pathology. In: Activit. nerv. sup. (Praha), Vol. 21, No 2, 105-115.
Schors, R. (1970): Leistungsmotivation und Aggressionsverhalten bei Hypertonikern. (Hamburg)
Seligman, M. (1975): Helplessness.
Vainstein,. I. Simonov, P. V. (1979): Emotiogenic Structures of Brain and Heart, (Moscow: Nauka Publishers), (in Russian)
Wolff, H. H. (1969): The Role of Aggression in the Psychopathology of Illness. In: J. Psycho-som. Res. Vol. 13, No 3, 315-320.